Скачать книгу

laboratory settings.

      Case studies: examine in depth one or more people with a certain characteristic.

      Surveys: series of questions to which people respond via a questionnaire or an interview.

      Description is typically the first step in conducting predictive and explanatory research. In the next section of this chapter, we will preview what are called descriptive statistics. In Chapters 2 through 5, we will look extensively at these types of statistics. Their overriding purpose is to help researchers describe data from a sample.

      Goal: To Predict

      Predictive research aims to make forecasts about future events. In the case of the weather, if we know the time of year it is, wind flow patterns, and barometric pressure, we can predict the temperature and likelihood of precipitation. Returning to our health-behaviors research, if we have data on college students’ health behaviors, we can use those data to predict outcomes such as grade-point average and satisfaction with college, both of which most colleges are keenly interested in. There are two methods of conducting predictive research. First, using the correlational method, researchers measure the extent to which two or more variables are related to each other (i.e., co-related). In our example, if we know how much sleep a college student gets each night, how many times per week he or she exercises, and his or her daily fruit and vegetable consumption, we can predict, to some extent, outcomes such as GPA and satisfaction with college.

Figure 13

      Figure 1.3 What a Positive Correlation Looks Like

      We will explore correlational research in more detail in Chapters 12 and 13. For now, understand that there are positive correlations, in which increases (or decreases) in the frequency of one behavior tend to be accompanied by increases (or decreases) in the frequency of a second behavior. To illustrate what a positive correlation looks like, consider Figure 1.3, which displays the nature of the relationship between weekly exercise habits and GPA (Bass, Brown, Laurson, & Coleman, 2013). Each dot on this scatterplot represents one student’s weekly aerobic exercise time (x-axis) and the student’s corresponding GPA (y-axis). In general, as weekly aerobic exercise time increases, GPA increases. This does not happen for every student, but in general, this is the case. Therefore, weekly aerobic exercise and GPA are positively correlated.

Figure 14

      Figure 1.4 What a Negative Correlation Looks Like

      In addition, the second type of correlation is a negative correlation, which results when increases in the frequency of one behavior tend to be accompanied by decreases in the frequency of a second behavior. To illustrate what a negative correlation looks like, consider Figure 1.4, which displays the hypothetical relationship between weekly alcohol consumption and GPA. In general, as weekly alcohol consumption increases, GPA decreases (Singleton & Wolfson, 2009).

      Finally, the third type of correlation is a zero correlation. As you might have guessed, a zero correlation exists when there is no pattern between the frequency of one behavior and the frequency of a second behavior. I am aware of no research that suggests any relationship between physical height and frequency of flossing one’s teeth. I doubt that taller people floss more often than shorter people (which would have been a positive correlation) or that shorter people floss more often than taller people (which would have been a negative correlation).

      In addition to the correlational method, predictive research makes use of quasi-experiments. A quasi-experiment compares naturally occurring groups of people. We could compare whether first-years, sophomores, juniors, or seniors have higher GPAs and higher levels of college satisfaction. Here, year-in-school is a quasi-independent variable in the sense that people tend to fall into one of these four types of students. Realize, as with correlational methods, we cannot conclude that being a senior causes students to do better or worse in school (or anything else) than being a first-year. However, such data could still be of interest. For instance, suppose we find the somewhat counterintuitive result that first-years earn higher GPAs and are more satisfied with college than are sophomores. College faculty and administrators would likely want to do more research to understand this relationship (and perhaps do something to facilitate the sophomore experience on their campuses).

      Prediction is more powerful than description. Although prediction is never perfect (just think about weather forecasts), it does provide insights into the world around us. Chapters 12 and 13 will provide us with tools that allow us to make predictions from our data.

      Predictive research: makes forecasts about future events.

      Correlational method: examines how and the extent to which two variables are related to each other.

      Positive correlation: increases (or decreases) in one variable tend to be accompanied by increases (or decreases) in a second variable. In other words, the two variables tend to relate in the same direction.

      Negative correlation: increases in one variable tend to be accompanied by decreases in a second variable. In other words, the two variables tend to relate in the opposite direction.

      Zero correlation: no relationship exists between two variables.

      Quasi-experiment: compares naturally existing groups, such as socioeconomic groups.

      Goal: To Explain

      Explanatory research takes descriptive and predictive research one step further; that is, explanatory research (also called experimental research) allows researchers to draw cause-and-effect conclusions between phenomena of interest. The researcher uses “control” to establish cause-and-effect conclusions. By “control” in the context of explanatory research, a researcher must manipulate (i.e., control) some aspect of behavior. The behavior that is controlled is called the independent variable. In this example, the independent variable is level of aerobic exercise. It is “independent” because the researchers can decide, within ethical boundaries, what to expose participants to in the experiment. It is called “variable” because some participants engaged in aerobic exercise, and others did not. Had all participants engaged in aerobic exercise, there would be nothing that varied. There must be at least two groups created by manipulating (controlling) an independent variable. Without at least two groups, you would have no way to make a comparison on how people’s behavior was affected.5

      Of course, we want to know an outcome of the independent variable. That outcome is called the dependent variable. That is, are there differences in academic performance based on whether people engaged in aerobic exercise? Such potential differences “depend” on the independent variable.

      You might well be wondering at this point how we can draw cause-and-effect conclusions from the independent variable’s effect on the dependent variable. Researchers use random assignment of participants in the sample either to engage in aerobic exercise or not to engage in aerobic exercise. Think about the many ways people differ from one another. For instance, I grew up during the relative economic boom years of the 1980s in the North Dallas suburbs, raised by parents from the northeastern United States. Such an upbringing may well differ from yours, likely in more than one way. And that’s just a couple of ways we might differ from each other. Not that such differences

Скачать книгу