Скачать книгу

affects parenting behaviors which, in turn, affect the child’s attachment style (van IJzendoorn, 1995).

       Parental relationship

      At a basic level, the presence of both parents in the home appears to be associated with attachment security (Booth‐LaForce et al., 2014); however, lower marital satisfaction (Howes & Markman, 1989), decreased marital harmony (Teti et al., 1995), and greater marital conflict (Owen & Cox, 1997) have been linked to greater attachment insecurity. Still, many studies have failed to replicate these results, suggesting a complex relation between parental marital functioning and a child’s attachment.

      Contextual Protective Factors

       Maternal sensitivity

      Many studies (see Belsky & Fearon, 2008) have documented a relation between a mother’s sensitivity to the needs of her infant and the infant’s attachment style. Specifically, a mother’s rapid response when her child is in distress (e.g., Del Carmen, Pederson, Huffman, & Bryan, 1993), as well as her warmth and involvement with the child (e.g., Leyendecker, Lamb, Fracasso, Scholmerich, & Larson, 1997) have been repeatedly linked to secure attachment in the child including across cultures and SES (Belsky & Fearon, 2008). However, meta‐analytic research on these relations reveals small effect sizes (De Wolff & van IJzendoorn, 1997; Belsky & Fearon, 2008). Two related concepts—maternal reflective functioning (Fonagy, Steel, & Steel, 1991) and maternal mind‐mindedness (Bernier & Dozier, 2003)—have garnered attention within attachment research. Both of these concepts are centered upon a parent’s ability to recognize their baby’s underlying mental states and link these mental states to the baby’s behavior. Research suggests that increases in this maternal capacity enhance attachment security in young children (Slade et al., 2005).

       Social support

      Gene–Environment Interplay

       Gene–environment interactions

      Several of the biological contributors to attachment that we have already discussed have been shown to impact attachment via interactions with environmental variables. Although researchers are careful to point out that gene–environment interaction studies in attachment are sparse, the prevailing view is that there are likely to be genetically based vulnerabilities that make children more or less susceptible to caregiving environments (Gervai, 2009). For example, a polymorphism of the serotonin transporter gene interacts with the caregiving environment to predict attachment insecurity (Spangler, Johann, Ronai, & Zimmerman, 2009). Similar findings have been noted with regard to a dopamine receptor gene; genetic vulnerability seems to increase the effects of difficult caregiving on attachment (van IJzendoorn & Bakermans‐Kranenburg, 2006) but also enhances the effects of a positive environment (Bakermans‐Kranenburg & van IJzendoorn, 2007), suggesting differential susceptibility to both favorable and unfavorable environments.

       Gene–environment correlations

      Developmental Timing Effects: Sensitive Periods

      Most attachment research has traditionally pointed to early infancy as an influential time in the parent–child attachment. Separations from primary caregivers that occur near six months of age appear to have a particularly detrimental impact on children’s attachment (Hazen et al., 2015). Still, it would be too simplistic to say that only the first six months of life are relevant for later attachment. Indeed, Fraley and Hefferman (2013) found that individuals who were younger (i.e., within the first few years of life) at the time that their parents divorced were more likely to have an insecure parental attachment in adulthood than individuals whose parents divorced later in childhood.

      Child–Parent Psychotherapy (CPP) is a manualized intervention for families with young children (less than five years of age; Lieberman & Van Horn, 2005) who have experienced trauma and helps parent resolve their own difficult childhood experiences. This program increases maternal empathy (Lieberman, Weston, & Pawl, 1991) and attachment security (Cicchetti, Rogosch, & Toth, 2006), and reduces psychopathology among children (Lieberman, Van Horn, & Ippen, 2005). Two other programs, the UCLA Family Development Project (e.g., Heinicke et al., 2006) and Minding the Baby (e.g., Slade et al., 2005), target mothers before the birth of their firstborn infants.

Schematic illustratration of the circle of Security.

      Source: Cooper, Hoffman, Marvin, & Powell, CircleofSecurity.org. 1998.

      1 Dykas, M.

Скачать книгу