Скачать книгу

adaptation to its relatively few syllables, which create many meaning mappings for any given syllable. For example, it would be inefficient to simply represent the phonological properties of spoken Mandarin because it contains homophones with different meanings. Hence, the Chinese orthography uses semantic radicals to represent the meanings of words directly. Thus, whereas using an alphabetic writing system (there is such a system, Pinyin) would result in a huge number of homophones, the character system usually identifies a particular morpheme. In contrast, English seems a poor match to a syllabary because its phonological complexity would create large numbers of syllables, and thus less efficiency than an alphabet.

      The variations across language and writing systems have important implications for reading science. Perfetti and Verhoeven (2017, Table 19.1) present an extended summary of reading development across languages. Some conclusions are specific to writing systems and languages (e.g., phoneme awareness is more important for learners of alphabetic than nonalphabetic writing); some are applicable broadly within a writing system (e.g., phoneme awareness in alphabetic orthographies is not dependent on mapping consistency); some apply across all writing systems (e.g., children’s linguistic awareness emerges first at the syllable level).

      Comparative research has also stimulated the extension of models of alphabetic reading to nonalphabetic reading. Li and Pollatsek (2020) presented an integrated model of word reading and eye movement control for Chinese, applying the Interactive Activation model (McClelland & Rumelhart, 1981) for word identification while also implementing word segmentation. Segmentation is needed because spaces separate characters but not words. PDP models have also been extended to reading Chinese (Yang et al., 2009; Zevin, 2019) and to morphological effects in Hebrew (Plaut & Gonnerman, 2000).

       The brain’s reading network (revisited)

      A universal brain reading network is strongly predicted by the fundamental principle of reading: that it converts systematic visual input (structured by writing systems) into language‐mediated meaning. Early comparisons confirmed this prediction across alphabetic languages, while also showing variation of the reading network in relation to consistency of letter‐phoneme mappings. English reading showed more use of a ventral pathway that includes the inferior temporal gyrus (IFG) compared with the more consistent Italian (Paulesu et al., 2000) and Spanish (Jamal et al., 2012). However, testing universality requires comparisons beyond alphabetic systems, and Chinese provides a high‐contrast comparison with alphabetic reading.

      Early neuroimaging studies of reading Chinese produced evidence for both a universal network and writing‐system specific variations (Bolger et al., 2005; Tan et al., 2005), as does a more recent review (Xu et al., 2019). Universal areas include the left fusiform gyrus, highlighting its function in coding orthography regardless of the visual forms and mapping levels. However, Chinese shows more bilateral activation in posterior areas that support visual‐orthographic processing and a less prominent role in some (but not all) studies for the inferior frontal gyrus. Another difference is the more prominent role of the left middle frontal gyrus (LMFG) in Chinese. The LMFG’s location near a motor area involved in handwriting (Exner’s area) suggests that its prominence in Chinese reading reflects an effect of character writing on character reading, consistent with the importance of writing practice in Chinese literacy instruction. Evidence for this comes from greater overlap of passive recognition and imagined writing in the LMFG for Chinese than for English (Cao & Perfetti, 2017). The greater writing practice in learning to read Chinese may help secure long‐term orthographic memories for characters, consistent with conclusions from behavioral research (McBride‐Chang, Chung et al., 2011). Although writing seems especially important in Chinese reading, a study by Nakamura et al. (2012) comparing French and Chinese on recognizing handwriting suggested the writing‐reading role of the LMFG is shared across writing systems.

       Disruptions in the word‐identification system (revisited)

      We should expect universal neural patterns associated with disruption in the word‐identification system for two related reasons: 1) the apparent existence of brain reading networks that include universal components; and 2) the language constraint that all writing systems map graphs to language. However, manifestations of word‐reading problems, including dyslexia, may vary with how the writing system makes demands on phonology. Such variation may depend on the level of phonological mapping – the grain size, phoneme or syllable (Wydell, 2019) – and the extent to which meaning encoded in morphology‐preserving orthography can compensate for a phonological deficit.

      Chinese provides both of these. It maps syllables rather than phonemes and it has meaning cues in its morphological orthography that may further reduce the demands of phonology. The abjads of Hebrew and Arabic and the alpha‐syllabaries make additional demands on orthographic‐morphological processing, seemingly without substantially reducing the demands of phonology. Indeed, Chinese seems to require a multiple‐cause model that includes nonphonological sources. Phonological problems are found (Ho et al., 2000), but so too are associations of reading problems with rapid naming and orthographic knowledge (Ho et al., 2002) and morphology (Shu et al., 2006). Underactivation in the left middle frontal gyrus in Chinese readers with dyslexia appears more common than in alphabetic readers with dyslexia (Siok et al., 2004). If the LMFG supports neural‐motor preparation for character writing as part of character recognition (Cao & Perfetti, 2017), this may suggest an orthographic factor in Chinese dyslexia.

      Visual‐orthographic processing challenges may be expected in Chinese, given the demands of learning around 3,000 characters in the first six years of school (see McBride‐Chang et al., this volume). Visual attention and copying skills have been found to predict reading ability of children in Hong Kong (Liu et al., 2015). In the multicause analysis, Chinese reading has phonological dyslexia, but fewer cases compared with alphabetic reading and even fewer cases with phonology as the only factor. Both visual‐orthographic processes and knowledge of Chinese compounding morphology may be important factors (McBride‐Chang, Lam et al., 2011). Interestingly, modeling of Chinese dyslexia (Yang et al., 2009; Zevin, 2019) suggested that either a morpho‐semantic or phonological disturbance produced wide‐ranging character reading problems in Chinese; in contrast, a semantic disturbance in English affected only identification of exception words.

Скачать книгу